How to distinguish a “scientoskeptic” from a “scientoenthusiast”? Psychometric properties and criteria for qualitative interpretation of the scores of the Views of Science Questionnaire in a Polish quota sample

Autor

  • Łukasz Jach University of Silesia

Słowa kluczowe:

attitudes towards science, psychology of science, scientistic worldview, social image of science, beliefs about science

Abstrakt

Background

The main aim of this study was to develop criteria for qualitative interpretation of the scores of the Views of Science Questionnaire (VoSQ), which is a tool for measuring the level of scientistic worldview. Another goal was to verify the psychometric properties of the tool in an adequately large and demographically diverse sample.

Participants and procedure

The study involved 1,119 participants aged 18 to 87 who filled in the Polish version of the VoSQ via the Internet. The obtained results were subjected to reliability analysis, confirmatory factor analysis and analyses aimed at developing criteria for the qualitative interpretation of both individual and group scores of the VoSQ scales.

Results

The CFA analysis showed a satisfactory level of fit of the VoSQ factor structure containing one higher-order factor and four sub-factors. The reliability of the tool scales was also satisfactory. The obtained results showed gender and age differences, but no differences related to the level of education. This information was used to develop the percentile-based criteria for the interpretation of the individual scores and the mean and standard deviation-based criteria for qualitative interpretation of the group scores.

Conclusions

The relationship between science and its social reception is becoming an increasingly important issue. The development of criteria for the qualitative interpretation of the results of the Views of Science Questionnaire makes it possible to use it as a tool for diagnosing attitudes towards science, displayed by both individuals and groups. This knowledge may be useful in improving the effectiveness of social implementation.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Bibliografia

Biddlestone, M., Green, R., & Douglas, K. M. (2020). Cultural orientation, power, belief in conspiracy theories, and intentions to reduce the spread of COVID‐19. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 663–673. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12397.

Brewer, P. R., & Ley, B. L. (2013). Whose science do you believe? Explaining trust in sources of scientific information about the environment. Science Communication, 35, 115–137. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/1075547012441691.

Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 149–154. https:// doi.org/10.1177/1745691617706516.

Edis, T. (2018). Cosmic conspiracy theories: How theologies evade science: From genesis to astrobiology. In J. Seckbach & R. Gordon (Eds.), Theology and science (pp. 143–165). World Scientific Publishing.

Escolar-Chua, R. L., & de Guzman, A. B. (2014) Effects of third age learning programs on the life satisfaction, self-esteem, and depression level among a select group of community dwelling Filipino elderly. Educational Gerontology, 40, 77–90, https://doi.org/ 10.1080/03601277.2012.701157.

Gauchat, G. (2011). The cultural authority of science: Public trust and acceptance of organized science. Public Understanding of Science, 20, 751–770. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662510365246.

Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO Reports, 11, 493–499. https://doi.org/10.1038/ embor.2010.84.

Gupta, N., Fischer, A. R., & Frewer, L. J. (2012). Socio-psychological determinants of public acceptance of technologies: a review. Public Understanding of Science, 21, 782–795. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0963662510392485.

GUS (2015). Wartości i zaufanie społeczne w Polsce w 2015 r. [Values and social trust in Poland in 2015]. Główny Urząd Statystyczny.

Harambam, J., & Aupers, S. (2015). Contesting epistemic authority: Conspiracy theories on the boundaries of science. Public Understanding of Science, 24, 466–480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662514559891.

Hendriks, F., Kienhues, D., & Bromme, R. (2016). Trust in science and the science of trust. In B. Blöbaum (Ed.), Progress in IS. Trust and communication in a digitized world: Models and concepts of trust research (pp. 143–159). Springer International Publishing.

Hauser, D., Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2019). Common concerns with MTurk as a participant pool. In F. R. Kardes, P. M. Herr, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in consumer psychology (pp. 319–337). Routledge.

Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2020). A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11, 1110–1118. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620934692.

Impey, C., Buxner, S., & Antonellis, J. (2012). Nonscientific beliefs among undergraduate students. Astronomy Education Review, 11. https://doi.org/ 10.3847/AER2012016.

Jach, Ł. (2015). Nauka jako obiekt kultu. Wprowadzenie do koncepcji scjentoteizmu [Science as an object of worship. Introduction to the theory of scientotheism]. Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego.

Jach, Ł. (2019). Spotlight on scientotheism. Structure and psychometic properties of the questionnaire for the study of scientistic worldview aspects. The Review of Psychology, 62, 141–165.

Jach, Ł. (2020). Światopogląd scjentystyczny – korelaty i uwarunkowania [Scientistic worldview – correlates and conditions]. Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Śląskiego.

Jach, Ł., & Chmiel, S. (2018). The reliability of advertising, the rule of social proof and the rule of scientific authority. Polish Journal of Economic Psychology, 13, 19–34. https://doi.org/10.15678/PJOEP.2018.13.02.

Kuntz, M. (2012). The postmodern assault on science. If all truths are equal, who cares what science has to say? EMBO Reports, 13, 885–889. https://doi. org/10.1038/embor.2012.130.

McDonough, C. C. (2016). The effect of ageism on the digital divide among older adults. Journal of Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, 2, 008. https://doi. org/10.24966/GGM-8662/100008.

Metz, S. E., Weisberg, D. S., & Weisberg, M. (2018), Non‐scientific criteria for belief sustain counter‐ scientific beliefs. Cognitive Science, 42, 1477–1503. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12584.

Motta, M. (2019). Explaining science funding attitudes in the United States: The case for science interest. Public Understanding of Science, 28, 161– 176. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662518795397.

Pinker, S. (2018). Enlightenment now: The case for reason, science, humanism, and progress. Penguin Books.

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17, 354–373. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029315.

Rull, V. (2014). The most important application of science: As scientists have to justify research funding with potential social benefits, they may well add education to the list. EMBO Reports, 15, 919– 922. https://doi.org/10.15252/embr.201438848.

Rutjens, B. T., Sutton, R. M., & van der Lee, R. (2018). Not all skepticism is equal: Exploring the ideological antecedents of science acceptance and rejection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 384–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616721774131.4

Seoyong, K., & Sunhee, K. (2015) The role of value in the social acceptance of science-technology. International Review of Public Administration, 20, 305– 322. https://doi.org/10.1080/12294659.2015.1078081.

Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124, 209–215. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.005.

Uscinski, J. E., Enders, A. M., Klofstad, C. A., Seelig, M. I., Funchion, J. R., Everett, C., Wuchty, S., Premaratne, K., & Murthi, M. N. (2020). Why do people believe COVID-19 conspiracy theories? The Harvard Kennedy School (HKS) Misinformation Review, 1. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-015.

Wessling, K. S., Huber, J., & Netzer, O. (2017). MTurk character misrepresentation: Assessment and solutions. Journal of Consumer Research, 44, 211–230. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx053.

Opublikowane

2021-03-01

Jak cytować

Jach, Łukasz. (2021). How to distinguish a “scientoskeptic” from a “scientoenthusiast”? Psychometric properties and criteria for qualitative interpretation of the scores of the Views of Science Questionnaire in a Polish quota sample. Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 9(1), 66–83. Pobrano z https://czasopisma.bg.ug.edu.pl/index.php/CIiPP/article/view/5766

Numer

Dział

Artykuły