Developmental Psychology in cultural historical context – overview and further reflections

Autor

  • Willem Koops Utrecht University

Słowa kluczowe:

history of childhood, infantilization, disappearance of childhood, historical developmental psychology

Abstrakt

From Bill Kessen’s idea of the child as a “cultural invention” (Kessen, 1983) it follows that developmental psychology cannot function fruitfully without historical analysis. Developmentalists should stop “positivistic dreaming” and develop a historical developmental psychology. The history of childhood shows how a historical process of infantilization has taken place since Rousseau and the 19th century pedagogical and educational theories and institutions. In the 20th century a new process of deinfantilization took place, caused mainly by the modern mass media (Postman, 1982). It is demonstrated how this led to the “disappearance of childhood”. Babies no longer were considered and studied as “empty-headed” (William James’ conception of the baby experiencing “one great blooming, buzzing confusion”): impressive new research methods and data “filled the baby’s brain” and made the baby much more human than ever before in history. With the narrowing of the gap between childhood and adulthood adolescence as a bridge is less necessary than before. Not only the disappearance of childhood is going on; at the same time there is a correlated disappearance of adolescence. The conclusion must be that the study of cognitive, social and personality development should take into consideration the cultural historical embeddedness.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Bibliografia

Adelson, J. (1979). Adolescence and the generation gap. Psychology Today, 12, 33–37.

Bandura, A. (1964). The stormy decade: Fact or fiction? Psychology in the Schools, 1, 224–231. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(196407)1:33.0.CO;2-I.

Dasen, P. (2000). Rapid social change and the turmoil of adolescence: a cross-cultural perspective. International Journal of Group Tensions, 29, 17–49. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005126629553.

Douvan, E., & Adelson, J. (1966). The adolescent experience. Wiley.

Epstein, R. (2010). Teen 2.0: Saving our children and families from the torment of adolescence. Quill Driver Books.

Freeman, D. (1983). Margaret Mead and Somoa: The making and unmaking of an anthropological myth. Penguin Books.

Freeman, D. (1998). The fateful hoaxing of Margaret Mead: a historical analysis of her Samoan research. Westview Press.

Fukuyama, F. (1989). The end of history? The National Interest, 16, 3–18.

Fukuyama, F. (1992). The end of history and the last man. Free Press.

Gopnik, A. (2009). The philosophical baby: What children’s minds tell us about truth, love, and the meaning of life. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its psychology and its relations to physiology, anthropology, sociology, sex, crime, religion, and education. Appleton.

Harris, P. L. (1977). The child as psychologist. VU Amsterdam.

James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. Henry Holt.

Kandel, D. B., & Lesser, G. S. (1972). Youth in two worlds. Jossey-Bass.

Kessen, W. (1983). The child and other cultural inventions. In F. S. Kessel & A. W. Siegel (Eds.), The child and other cultural inventions (pp. 26–39). Praeger.

Koops, W., & Kessel, F. (2017). Developmental psychology without positivistic pretentions: an introduction to the special issue on historical developmental psychology. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 629–646. https://doi.org/10.1080/17 405629.2017.1382344.

Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa: a psychological study of primitive youth for Western civilization. William Morrow & Company.

Meeus, W. (1994). Adolescentie: een psychosociale benadering [Adolescence: a psychosocial approach]. Wolters-Noordhoff.

Meeus, W. (2019). Adolescent development: Longitudinal research into the self, personal relationships, and psychopathology. Routledge.

Offer, D. (1969). The psychological world of the teenager. Basic Books.

Popper, K. (1935). Logik der Forschung [The logic of research]. Julius Springer Verlag.

Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. Hutchinson.

Popper, K. (1963). Conjectures and refutations. Routlegde.

Postman, N. (1982) The disappearance of childhood. Vintage Books.

Prins, F. W. (1963). Verleden en heden: Beknopte geschiedenis van opvoeding en onderwijs [Past and present: Concise history of upbringing and education]. Wolters.

Reble, A. (1965). Johann Bernhard Basedow. Schöningh.

Rousseau, J. J. (1763). Émile on education. Nourse and Vaillant [Originally published as Émile ou de l’éducation, 1762].

Rutter, M., Graham, P., Chadwick, O., & Yule, W. (1976). Adolescent turmoil: Fact or fiction? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 35–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00372.x.

Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1991). Adolescence: an anthropological inquiry. Free Press.

Wellman, H. W. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. MIT Press. Wellman, H. W. (2015). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford University Press.

White, S. H. (1994). G. Stanley Hall: From philosophy to developmental psychology. In R. D. Parke, P. A. Ornstein, J. J. Rieser, & C. Zahn-Waxler (Eds.), A century of developmental psychology (pp. 103– 127). APA Publishing.

Opublikowane

2023-08-02

Jak cytować

Koops, W. (2023). Developmental Psychology in cultural historical context – overview and further reflections. Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 11(2), 130–136. Pobrano z https://czasopisma.bg.ug.edu.pl/index.php/CIiPP/article/view/9192

Numer

Dział

Artykuły