Emotionality and the assessment of memory performance (one’s own and that of other people)

Autor

  • Ewa Małgorzata Szepietowska Uniwersytet Marii Curie-Skłodowskiej

Słowa kluczowe:

memory self-efficacy, memory efficacy in others, negative and positive state-trait affect

Abstrakt

Background
In clinical practice, the assessments provided by relatives are treated as a source of information about the emotional-cognitive difficulties manifested by a customer. Usually, those opinions are treated as objective data, i.e. data that indicate the real possibilities of the evaluated person.
Data suggest that personality and affective factors of the assessing individual may modify the manner of describing an individual person in one’s care, regardless of the actual difficulties experienced by that individual.

Participants and procedure
In the study, the scale of the Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ) was used in the version investigating oneself, a peer and an elderly individual in relation to whom the assessing individual undertakes care and support activities. The MFQ is relevant to different aspects of memory. The MFQ is composed of 64 questions grouped in scales composed of different numbers of items. Low results on the scales and a low general result illustrate the feeling that one experiences memory difficulties, whereas higher results illustrate the feeling that one’s memory is highly efficient.
The participants (N = 65 individuals; 61% female and 29% male) were between 40 and 76 years old (M = 51.50 and SD = 7.25). In the analyses, the intensity of depression (GDS) and of positive and negative emotionality – state and trait (SUPIN) – of the assessing individual were taken into consideration, assuming that their intensity would correlate with opinions relevant to mnemonic capacities (one’s own and those of other people).

Results
On average, the participants assess their mnemonic competence as higher than that of their peers and of the elderly. The higher the level of abilities which the former ascribe to themselves, the better are those which are ascribed by them to a peer and an elderly individual. Negative affectivity shapes the conviction that one’s own mnemonic competence is deteriorating, and that the same thing is happening to that of a peer/an elderly individual as well.

Conclusions
Evaluation of the psychological functioning of a customer prepared by relatives/a caregiver can indicate symptoms of subclinical severity.
There exists a connection between the affective-personality traits of a caregiver and the manner of the assessment of mnemonic competence (one’s own and that of near relations). The direction of those connections is not clear. The negative affectivity of a near relation/caregiver may result in formulating negative opinions about oneself, and about those in their care. Conversely, typical developmental memory changes in oneself and others can cause anxiety and depressive mood.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Bibliografia

Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729-735.

Barrett, R. D., McLellan, T., & McKinley, A. (2013). Self versus family ratings of the frontal systems behaviour scale and measures executive functions; adult outcomes following childhood traumatic brain injury. PLoS One, 8, e76916. DOI: 10.1371/ journal.pone.0076916.

Blazer, D. G. (2002). Self-efficacy and depression in late life: A primary prevention proposal. Aging & Mental Health, 6, 315-324.

Brzozowski, P. (2010). Skala uczuć pozytywnych i negatywnych (SUPIN): Polska adaptacja skali PANAS Dawida Watsona i Lee Anny Clark. Podręcznik [The scale of positive and negative feelings (SUPIN): Polish adaptation of the PANAS scale by David Watson and Lee Anna Clark. Textbook]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.

Cahn-Weiner, D., Ready, R., & Malloy P. (2003). Neuropsychological predictors of everyday memory and everyday functioning in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease. http://www.nwpsych.com/cfmal/ Guardianship%20related%20references-1-11-10/ Cahn-Weiner%202003%20NP%20predictors%20 of%20memory%20and%20ADL%20in%20mild%20 AD.pdf.

Cherry, K., Brigman, S., Hawley, K., & Reese, C. (2003). The Knowledge of Memory Aging Questionnaire: effects of adding a “don’t know” response option. Educational Gerontology, 29, 427-446.

Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & Hertzog, C. (1988). The Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 671-688.

Eich, T., Murayama, K., Castel, A., & Knowlton, B. (2014). The dynamic effects of age-related stereotype threat on explicit and implicit memory performance in older adults. Social Cognition, 32, 559-570.

Eriksen, H., & Ihlebaek, C. (2002). Subjective health complaints. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 101-103.

Forgas, J., Bower, G., & Krantz, S. (1984). The influences of mood on perceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 497-513.

Frank, L., Lenderking, W., Howard, K., & Cantillon, M. (2011). Patient self-report for evaluating mild cognitive impairment and prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 3, 35.

Fritsch, Th., McClendon, M., Wallendal, M., Hyde, T., & Larsen, J. (2014). Prevalence and cognitive bases of subjective memory complaints in older adults: evidence from a community sample. Hindawi Publishing Corporation Journal of Neurodegenerative Diseases. Article ID 176843. http://dx.doi. org/10.1155/2014/176843.

Gilewski, M. J., & Zelinski, E. M. (1986). Questionnaire assessment of memory complaints. In: L. W. Poon (ed.), The handbook of clinical memory as sessment of older adults (pp. 93-107). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Gilewski, M. J., Zelinski, E. M., & Schaie, K. W. (1990). The Memory Functioning Questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in adulthood and old age. Psychology and Aging, 5, 482-490.

Hart, T., Whyte, J., Polansky, M., Millis, S., Hammond, F., Sherer, M., Bushnik, T., Hanks, R., & Kreutzer, J. (2003). Concordance of patient and family report of neurobehavioral symptoms at 1 year after traumatic brain injury. Archives Physical Medical Rehabilitation, 84, 204-213.

Holm, S., Schӧnberger, M., Poulsen, I., & Caetano, C. (2009). Patients’ and relatives’ experience of difficulties following severe traumatic brain injury: The sub-acute stage. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 19, 444-460.

Kalpouzos, G., & Eriksson, J. (2013). Memory self-efficacy beliefs modulate brain activity when encoding real-world future intentions. PLoS One, 8, e73850. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073850.

Kavanagh, D., & Bower, G. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 507-525.

Lima-Silva, T., & Yassuda, M. (2009). The relationship between memory complaints and age in normal aging. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 3, 94-100.

Roessler-Górecka, M., Iwański, S., & Seniów, J. (2013). The value of self-report methods in neuropsychological diagnostics of patients after brain injury. Psychiatria Polska, 3, 465-472.

Sink, K. M., Covinsky, K. E., Barnes, D. E., Newcomer, R. J., & Yaffe, K. (2006). Caregiver characteristics are associated with neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia. Journal of American Geriatric Society, 54, 796-803.

Spruytte, N., Audenhove, Ch., Lammertyn, F., & Storms, G. (2002). The quality of the caregiving relationship in informal care for older adults with dementia and chronic psychiatry patients. Psychology and Psychotherapy, Research and Practice, 75, 295-311.

van Heugten, C., Rasquin, S., Winkens, I., Beusmans, G., & Verhey, F. (2007). Checklist for cognitive and emotional consequences following stroke (CLCE24): development, usability and quality of the self-report version. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 109, 257-262.

Vanderploeg, R., Brlanger, H., Duchnick, J., & Curtiss, G. (2007). Awareness problems following moderate to severe traumatic brain injury: prevalence, assessment methods, and injury correlates. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 44, 937-950.

van Rijsbergen, M., Mark, R., de Kort, P., & Sitskoorn, M. (2013). The COMPlaints after stroke (COMPAS) study: protocol for a Dutch cohort study on poststroke subjective cognitive complaints. British Medical Journal Open, 3, e003599. DOI: 10.1136/ bmjopen-2013-003599.

van Rijsbergen, M., Mark, R., de Kort, P., & Sitskoom, M. (2015). Prevalence and profile of poststroke subjective cognitive complaints. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases, 24, 1823-1831.

West, R. L., Dennehy-Basile, D., & Norris, M. P. (1996). Memory self-evaluation: the effects of age and experience. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 3, 67-83.

Yesavage, J., Brink, T., Rose, T., Lum., O., Huakg, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, O. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 17, 37-49.

Zanardo, F., De Beni, R., & Moè A. (2006). Influence of other-beliefs on self-beliefs and on everyday memory self-report in the elderly. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 18, 425-432. DOI: 10.1007/BF03324839.

Zelinski, E. M., & Gilewski, M. J. (2004). A 10-item Rasch modeled memory self-efficacy scale. Aging & Mental Health, 8, 293-306.

Opublikowane

2015-09-01

Jak cytować

Szepietowska, E. M. (2015). Emotionality and the assessment of memory performance (one’s own and that of other people). Current Issues in Personality Psychology, 3(3), 139–148. Pobrano z https://czasopisma.bg.ug.edu.pl/index.php/CIiPP/article/view/12229

Numer

Dział

Artykuły